Research Ideas and Outcomes : Research Idea
|
Corresponding author: Andrey Vyshedskiy (vysha@bu.edu)
Received: 28 Dec 2015 | Published: 29 Dec 2015
© 2016 Andrey Vyshedskiy, Rita Dunn.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation: Vyshedskiy A, Dunn R (2015) Mental synthesis involves the synchronization of independent neuronal ensembles. Research Ideas and Outcomes 1: e7642. doi: 10.3897/rio.1.e7642
|
Imagine an apple on top of a dolphin. Visualizing this new, never-before-seen image in your mind’s eye involves a purposeful synthesis of two familiar mental images tucked away in your brain. Let’s call this process mental synthesis. Mental synthesis is a key component of what we commonly call ‘imagination,’ along with other components such as simple memory recall, spontaneous insight, dreaming, and hallucination. Mental synthesis is one of the least understood and also one of the most interesting components of imagination, since it is responsible for so many of the uniquely human traits, such as mental planning, modeling and engineering. Humans are able to purposefully and deliberately create and inspect a seemingly endless array of novel images in the mind’s eye, but the neurological process underlying this essential human skill is not well understood.
This manuscript proposes an experiment aimed at testing the hypothesis that, neurologically, mental synthesis is mediated by the synchronization of two or more independent neuronal ensembles.
mental synthesis, imagination, binding-by-synchrony, independent neuronal ensembles, neuronal ensemble synchronization, neuronal synchronization, evolution of language
What happens in one’s brain when two objects, never before seen together (say, an apple on top of a dolphin), are imagined together for the first time? The scientific consensus is that a familiar object, such as an apple or a dolphin, is encoded in the brain by a network of neurons known as a neuronal ensemble (
The synchronization mechanism of mental synthesis is likely responsible for many imaginative and creative traits that philosophers and scientists have recognized as being uniquely human, despite not having a precise neurological understanding of the process. For example, Lev Vygotsky claims, “Imagination is a new formation that is not present in the consciousness of the very young child, is totally absent in animals, and represents a specifically human form of conscious activity” (
Mental synthesis is also an integral part of human language. When we speak we use mental synthesis to describe a novel image (“My house is the second one on the left, just across the road from the red gate”) and we rely on the listener to use mental synthesis in order to visualize the novel image. Mental synthesis is essential for understanding common elements of language, such as: flexible syntax (“My friend ate a fish” vs. “A fish ate my friend” – notice that you have to mentally synthesize two disparate novel images involving your friend and a fish in order to correctly understand the meaning of the two statements), verb tenses (“My friend ate a fish” vs. “My friend was eaten by a fish”), and spatial prepositions (“put a bowl behind a cup” vs. “put a bowl in front of the cup”). Flexible syntax, verb tenses, prepositions, adjectives, and other common elements of grammar facilitate the human ability to communicate an infinite number of novel images with the use of a finite number of words – the quality of language that linguists refer to as “recursion.” Mental synthesis is acquired by children uniformly across all cultures around the age of three along with a culturally determined vocabulary. Understanding the basis of mental synthesis can shed light on the evolution of the brain in general and on the evolution of language in particular.
What are the neurological bases of mental synthesis? Neuronal ensembles encoding sensory memories are largely located in the posterior cortex (temporal, parietal and occipital lobes), while the temporal organization of behavior is primarily a function of the frontal cortex (
The Mental Synthesis Theory can be tested in a conceptually simple experiment using the established technique of single-cell recording in patients undergoing treatment for epilepsy.
Over the past two decades, considerable excitement was generated within the neuroscience community by experiments which recorded the neuronal activity of fully conscious and responsive human patients (
Recoding from hundreds of patients demonstrated that most neurons in the temporal lobe are object-selective. That is, a given neuron fires at a high rate only when the patient perceives a specific object, such as an animal, a baseball, the Sydney Opera House, or Bill Clinton, and not when any other object is perceived (
It is important to note that the Bill Clinton neuron is not the only neuron activated when a patient perceives Bill Clinton. Likely it is one of thousands of neurons encoding the perception of Bill Clinton; it just happens to be the neuron that the scientists are recording from. There is significant evidence that the perception of every object is associated with the synchronous firing of thousands of neurons located throughout the brain (for review see
Now that we have reviewed the neurology behind the perception of a familiar object, let’s return to the question raised in this manuscript: what happens when two objects that have never been seen together are imagined together for the first time? Such integration or mental synthesis occurs routinely in our mind, however the mechanism of this process is unclear. To answer this question, we propose the following experiment, which is an extension of the scientific paradigm described above. Two or more very selective neurons encoding disparate objects would be identified in patients with implanted electrodes: for example, a neuron that is part of the Bill Clinton neuronal ensemble, and another neuron that is part of the neuronal ensemble of a lion. Let us call them the Clinton neuron and the lion neuron for the sake of brevity. Whenever the patient recalls Bill Clinton, the Clinton neuron we are recording from would fire at a high rate. Similarly when the patient recalls a lion, the lion neuron would fire at a high rate. The proposed experiment, however, would record what happens when the patient imagines these two objects together, for example: Bill Clinton holding the lion in his lap,
Mental synthesis of Bill Clinton holding a lion. Once selective neurons for Bill Clinton and the lion are identified, a subject can be asked to imagine Bill Clinton holding the lion on his lap. The Mental Synthesis theory predicts that both the Clinton neuron and the lion neuron will increase their firing rate and that their activity will be synchronized.
Images modified from: 1. William J. Clinton at the Parliament in London, United Kingdom, November 29, 1995. https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Bill_Clinton_1995_im_Parlament_in_London.jpg 2. Lioness in the Olomouc Zoo at Svatý kopeček, Czech Republic. This image is licensed under the CC BY-SA license. https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Lioness,_Olomouc.jpg
On a neurological level, mentally forming the image of Bill Clinton and the lion consists of the following steps:
Step 1 - Recall of Bill Clinton: The prefrontal cortex (PFC) activates the ensemble of neurons representing Bill Clinton to fire synchronous actions potentials. Bill Clinton is perceived by the patient. The electrode implanted into the temporal lobe (TL) records an increased rate of action potentials.
Step 2 - Recall of the lion: The PFC activates the ensemble of neurons representing the lion to fire synchronous actions potentials. The lion is perceived. The second electrode implanted into the TL records an increased rate of action potentials.
Step 3 - The patient mentally integrates the images of Bill Clinton and the lion into one scene. The Mental Synthesis Theory hypothesizes that integration is accomplished by the PFC synchronizing the two neuronal ensembles in time.
Step 4 - When synchronization of the Clinton and the lion neuronal ensembles is achieved, a new, never-before-seen mental image of Bill Clinton holding the lion on his lap is perceived by the patient. At that moment the two implanted electrodes are predicted to record synchronous action potentials, implying the synchronization of the Clinton and lion neuronal ensembles.
In addition to the visualization task which would give unprecedented insight into the nature of imagination, patients could also be shown an actual image of Bill Clinton holding the lion on his lap. Again, we would expect to see both the Clinton neuron as well as the lion neuron increase their firing rate and for their action potentials to synchronize.
Since researchers can often identify several object-selective neurons within a single patient, multiple novel pairings of objects can be studied. Furthermore, morphing of more than two objects into one mental frame can also be investigated. For example, if researchers happen to identify selective neurons for Bill Clinton, the Sydney Opera house, and a lion, the subject can be asked to imagine Bill Clinton sitting next to the Sydney Opera house and holding the lion. In this case, all three neurons would be expected to fire synchronously. This experimental paradigm also paves the way for many other interesting experiments studying the neuroscience of imagination. For example, what would happen on the neuronal level if: Bill Clinton was imagined as a statue rather than as a human being?; the lion was seen fighting Bill Clinton, rather than sitting on his lap?; the subject was to imagine the lion swallowing Bill Clinton?; etc.
In this manuscript we have proposed a conceptually simple experiment aimed at understanding the neurological mechanism of mental synthesis. Recording from electrodes implanted into the brains of patients with intractable epilepsy provides an opportunity to test the hypothesis that mental synthesis, the neurological phenomenon that underlies most of human creativity and imagination, involves the synchronization of independent neuronal ensembles. The technical implementation of this experiment would be challenging but not impossible. It would involve identifying multiple neurons within a particular patient that fire in response to specific objects (as has been done in multiple previous experiments), and then it would require the patient to successfully generate a stable image of those objects integrated in their mind’s eye.
The importance of such an experiment cannot be overstated. Since mental synthesis is likely a uniquely human faculty, understanding the neurological mechanism of mental synthesis will provide insights into how this ability evolved and, therefore, shed light on human evolution in general and the evolution of language in particular.